Mechanisms underlying clinical manifestations of treatment resistance in schizophrenia

Antipsychotics are not effective in 30% of patients with schizophrenia. These patients have treatment-resistant schizophrenia (TRS). Hypotheses for the neurobiological mechanisms implicated in TRS include dopamine supersensitivity and normal striatal dopamine neurotransmission. Experts from the UK and Canada provided a comprehensive overview of current understanding of the neurobiology for TRS, considered how dopamine supersensitivity might be prevented or attenuated, and highlighted the need for new treatment targets at a symposium that had ECNP delegates crowding to get into a packed auditorium.

National and international treatment guidelines are broadly aligned on the definition of TRS,1–3 said Oliver Howes, Professor of Molecular Psychiatry, King’s College London, UK. The common definition is no significant improvement in target symptoms after treatment with at least two different antipsychotics at an adequate dose and duration.

Need for novel treatments with non-dopamine D2 mechanisms of action

All licensed antipsychotic drugs act through antagonism (or partial agonism) at the dopamine D2 receptor,4 Professor Howes explained. However, multiple antipsychotic trials are not effective for approximately one-third of patients.5 It is therefore clear that a different approach to treatment is required, he said.

For 10–23% of people with schizophrenia, antipsychotics are never effective

Non-response may be primary, from the first episode, or secondary. Rates of primary treatment resistance range from 10–23%.5–8

Patients for whom dopamine D2 blockers are not effective may have a normodopaminergic profile, said Professor Howes, with normal striatal dopamine synthesis and release capacity1 and normal dopaminergic synapse density.9–12

In contrast, patients for whom dopamine D2 blockers are effective have a hyperdopaminergic profile with elevated striatal dopamine synthesis and release capacity,1 and a higher density of dopaminergic synapses.9–12

Professor Howes suggested that other neurotransmitter systems might also be implicated in TRS, including glutamate.13,14 Glutamate levels are significantly higher in patients with TRS compared to healthy volunteers,13 and patients with schizophrenia for whom D2 antagonists are effective.14

Other neurotransmitter systems that might be implicated in TRS include glutamate

Professor Howes also noted that other factors could contribute to TRS including D2/3 upregulation,15 neurodegeneration,16 reduced brain connectivity,17,18 substance misuse,19 genetic polymorphisms,20 neuroinflammation,21 abnormally low levels of cannabinoid receptor 1,22 and white matter integrity.23

Animal models of dopamine supersensitivity

Chronic antipsychotic treatment leads to changes in the brain, said Anne-Noël Samaha, Associate Professor of Pharmacology, University of Montreal, Canada, and – in animal models --some of these changes result in treatment tolerance. A possible reason for this is that prolonged antipsychotic exposure leads to compensatory changes and brain supersensitivity to dopamine receptor stimulation, resulting in exaggerated responses to dopamine.24,25

Such supersensitivity has been demonstrated in rats,26 Dr Samaha explained. The loss of efficacy occurred despite clinically relevant levels of dopamine D2/3 receptor occupancy and was not linked to changes in dopamine release or availability; but it was accompanied by upregulation of D2/3 receptors.26

Supersensitivity was more obvious with the strong D2 blockade achieved by typical antipsychotics, which bind tightly to the receptor. It was less evident with atypicals, when less tight binding allows for some endogenous dopamine transmission.

How can we prevent dopamine supersensitivity?

Intermittent antipsychotic treatment in a rat model does not cause dopamine supersensitivity27

Dr Samaha commented that in other words less may be more and that transient but regular antipsychotic treatment strategies need to be investigated further.

Once it has developed, antipsychotic-induced dopamine supersensitivity has been shown to be attenuated by:

  • pharmacological antagonism of 5-HT2A receptors28 — Dr Samaha suggested that this may be another explanation as to why atypical antipsychotics are less likely to lead to dopamine supersensitivity than typical antipsychotics
  • injecting neurotensin into the nucleus accumbens29 — activation of neurotensin receptors in the nucleus accumbens decreases dopamine affinity for D2 receptors and promotes D2 receptor internalization

5-HT2A antagonism attenuates dopamine supersensitivity

Are existing treatments ineffective because they target the wrong processes?

TRS differs from treatment-responsive schizophrenia clinically and biologically,30 said Ofer Agid, Associate Professor of Psychiatry, Toronto, Canada, and potentially there are different types of TRS,31,32 with different neurobiologies, psychopathologies, and clinical courses.33,34

Current antipsychotics do not improve symptoms for patients with TRS

Professor Agid highlighted how current antipsychotics do not significantly improve symptoms for patients with TRS as demonstrated by:

  • An analysis of 65 studies that showed that antipsychotics were ineffective in 60% of patients after 23 weeks.35
  • A substantial decline in antipsychotic effectiveness (from 75% to 17%) after the first trial in patients with first-episode schizophrenia.30
  • Antipsychotic effectiveness in only 7–9% of patients after two antipsychotics have been ineffective.36
  • The persistence of psychotic symptoms in 30–60% of patients.37

Antipsychotic effectiveness also declines with each relapse.38,39

Professor Agid described the continuum and categorical hypotheses for TRS.34 According to the continuum hypothesis, the same pathophysiological processes underlie symptoms in patients for whom antipsychotics are effective and ineffective, but occur to a greater degree in TRS. In contrast, the categorical hypothesis states that TRS has a fundamentally different pathophysiology from that of responsive schizophrenia, and existing treatments are ineffective because they target the wrong processes.

Professor Agid concluded that a hybrid of both the continuum and categorical hypotheses might best describe the neurobiology of TRS.

Educational financial support for this Symposium was provided by H. Lundbeck A/S

References
  1. Lehman et al. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161(2 Suppl):1–56.
  2. NICE Clinical Guideline 178, 2014.
  3. Hasan et al. World J Biol Psychiatry 2012;13:318–78.
  4. Leucht et al. Am J Psychiatry 2017;174(10):927–42.
  5. Lally et al. Psychol Med 2016;46(15):3231–40.
  6. Lieberman. J Clin Psychiatry 1999;60(Suppl)12:9–12.
  7. Robinson et al. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:544–9.
  8. Demjaha et al. Psychol Med 2017;47:1981–9.
  9. Howes & Kapur. Br J Psychiatry 2014;205(1):1–3.
  10. Roberts et al. Synapse 2009;63(6):520–30.
  11. Demjaha et al. Am J Psychiatry 2012;169(11):1203–10.
  12. Abi-Dargham et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97(14):8104–9.
  13. Demjaha et al. Biol Psychiatry 2014;75(5):e11–e13.
  14. Mouchlianitis et al. Schizophr Bull 2016;42(3):744–52.
  15. Silvestri et al. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2000;152(2):174–80.
  16. Anderson et al. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2015;18(7):pyv016.
  17. Alonso-Solís et al. Schizophr Res 2015;161(2–3):261–8.
  18. White et al. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016;41(5):1274–85.
  19. Picci et al. Psychiatry Res 2013;210(3):780–6.
  20. Zhang et al. Schizophr Res 2013;146(1–3):285–8.
  21. Mondelli et al. Schizophr Bull 2015;41(5):1162–70.
  22. Rangananthan et al. Biol Psychiatry 2016;79(12):997–1005.
  23. Reis Marques et al. Brain 2014;137(Pt 1):172–82.
  24. Chouinard et al. Am J Psychiatry 1978;135(11):1409–10.
  25. Fallon & Dursun. J Psychopharmacol 2011;25(6):755–62.
  26. Samaha et al. J Neurosci 2007;27(11):2979–86.
  27. Samaha et al. Biol Psychiatry 2008;64(2):145–52.
  28. Charron et al. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2015;25(12):2381–93.
  29. Servonnet et al. Neuropharmacology 2017;123:10–21.
  30. Agid et al. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2013;23(9):1017–22.
  31. Gillespie et al. BMC Psychiatry 2017;17(1):12.
  32. Farooq et al. Schizophr Bull 2013;39(6):1169–72.
  33. Iasevoli et al. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2016;65:34–48.
  34. Mouchlianitis et al. Lancet Psychiatry 2016;3(5):451–63.
  35. Kennedy et al. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 2014;29(2):63–76.
  36.  Kinon et al. Psychopharmacol Bull 1993;29(2):309–14.
  37. Lieberman et al. J Clin Psychiatry 1999;60(Suppl 12):9–12.
  38. Agid et al. Neuropsychopharmacology 2014;39:S373–4.
  39. Zipursky et al. Poster at ACNP 2014.
Country selection
We are registering that you are located in Brazil - if that's correct then please continue to Progress in Mind Brazil
You are leaving Progress in Mind
Hello
Please confirm your email
We have just sent you an email, with a confirmation link.
Before you can gain full access - you need to confirm your email.
The information on this site is exclusively intented for health care professionals.
All the information included in the Website is related to products of the local market and, therefore, directed to health professionals legally authorized to prescribe or dispense medications with professional practice. The technical information of the drugs is provided merely informative, being the responsibility of the professionals authorized to prescribe drugs and decide, in each concrete case, the most appropriate treatment to the needs of the patient.
Congress
Register for access to Progress in Mind in your country